Dr. David Linden: Life, Death & the Neuroscience of Your Unique Experience
Listen or watch on your favorite platforms
In this episode, my guest is Dr. David Linden, Ph.D., professor of neuroscience at Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine and the author of many popular books about the brain. We discuss individual differences between people — focusing on differences in how people sense the world around them and the roles that chance, heredity, and life experiences (even in utero) play in determining our physical and cognitive traits. We discuss the bidirectional connection between the mind and body and how our thoughts and mental practices (e.g., meditation and breathwork) impact our health. We also discuss the link between inflammation and depression. We also discuss Dr. Linden’s terminal illness diagnosis, his mindset during chemotherapy and what his diagnosis has taught him about the mind, gratitude, time perception and life. This episode also covers sensual touch, cerebellar function, and epigenetic inheritance and ought to be of interest to all interested in neuroscience, genetics, psychology and human development.
Articles
- Ben Barres (Nature)
- A Neuroscientist Prepares for Death (The Atlantic)
- Can a Neuroscientist Fight Cancer With Mere Thought? (The New York Times)
Books
Other Resources
- Dr. Leslie Vosshall
- Dr. Catherine Dulac
- Dr. Rachel Herz
- Dr. Karl Deisseroth
- Dr. Gloria Choi
- Dr. Irving Zucker’s Publications
- Dr. Rena Malik: Improving Sexual & Urological Health in Males and Females
- Dr. Oded Rechavi: Genes & the Inheritance of Memories Across Generations
- Dr. Eddie Chang: The Science of Learning & Speaking Languages
- Dr. Robin Carhart-Harris: The Science of Psychedelics for Mental Health
- Dr. Matthew Johnson: Psychedelic Medicine
About this Guest
Dr. David Linden
This transcript version is not in its final form and will be updated.
Andrew Huberman:
Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast, where we discuss science and science based tools for everyday life.
[OPENING THEME MUSIC]
I'm Andrew Huberman, and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology at Stanford School of Medicine. Today, my guest is Dr. David Linden. Dr. David Linden is a professor of neuroscience at Johns Hopkins School of Medicine. His laboratory has studied neuroplasticity. That is how connections in the brain change in response to experience. Much of that work focused on a structure called the cerebellum, which is also sometimes referred to as the mini brain because it looks like a mini brain in the bottom and back of the human brain. And it's responsible for an enormous number of basic functions that we use in everyday life including our motor behavior, that is our ability to walk and talk, but also dance, play instruments. And it's responsible for an enormous number of basic functions that we use in everyday life, including our sense of balance, our ability to learn new motor behaviors, as well as our sense of timing.
Today, we will discuss the cerebellum and what it does. But Dr. David Linden will also teach us about the important sense of touch, as well as what makes us different as individuals. The reason today's discussion encompasses so many important topics is that Dr. David Linden's laboratory has focused on many of those topics, and he is also the author of five excellent popular books about neuroscience that focus on, for instance, our sense of pleasure and where it originates from and what controls it in the brain, as well as our sense of touch. And today, we start off our discussion by talking about the recent discovery of a set of neurons that have been known about for a long period of time, but that only recently have been characterized, that are involved in sensual touch in particular. And it's a fascinating conversation, I assure you.
In addition to that, Dr. David Linden informs us about what makes us individuals, how each and every one of us perceives the same things differently. And it's an absolutely fascinating conversation which tells you, for instance, why some of you think a smell is putrid, indeed smells like vomit, whereas others, perhaps are not bothered by that smell, and why others still are attracted to that smell or something that you look at or something that you hear. We also talk about nature versus nurture and how we come to be who we are, not just through our genes and epigenetics, but also through our early childhood experience and adult experience.
And then in the latter third of our conversation, we shift to talking about the so called mind-body connection and the science underlying how our thoughts inform our bodily health, or lack thereof. As well as how the organs of our body control the chemicals, hormones, and thoughts within our brain. Then we shift to discussing Dr. David Linden himself and the fact that in 2020, he was diagnosed with a form of heart cancer that led his physicians to tell him that he had six to twelve months to live.
Now, obviously, because he was in our studio to record this conversation, he has outlived that prognosis. But he lives day to day with the knowledge that his death may very well come soon, although it isn't clear exactly when that day will come, of course. He tells us how the initial prognosis of his cancer, as well as outliving that prognosis, has informed his day to day life, as well as his thinking and his relationships. And that leads to a very direct and frankly, emotional conversation that includes advice on how all of us can get the most out of our daily living and out of our overall life. It's an extremely powerful conversation that I believe everyone, regardless of age or health status, can benefit from. And it's one that makes clear that not only is Dr. David Linden a spectacular scientist, but also a spectacular educator, a spectacular popular writer, a spectacular family man, including husband and father and friend to many people and his colleagues, but he is also a courageous and spectacularly generous human being.
Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize that this podcast is separate from my teaching and research roles at Stanford. It is, however, part of my desire and effort to bring zero cost to consumer information about science and science related tools to the general public.
In keeping with that theme, I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast. Our first sponsor is ROKA. ROKA makes eyeglasses and sunglasses that are of the absolute highest quality. The company was founded by two all American swimmers from Stanford, and everything about ROKA eyeglasses and sunglasses were designed with performance in mind. I've spent a lifetime working on the biology of the visual system, and I can tell you that your visual system has to contend with an enormous number of challenges in order for you to be able to see clearly. ROKA understands those challenges and the biology of the visual system such that they've designed sunglasses and eyeglasses that always allow you to see with crystal clarity.
Now, initially, ROKA eyeglasses and sunglasses were designed for sports performance, and as a consequence, all of their glasses are designed to be very lightweight and to not slip off your face if you get sweaty. However, the design of the glasses include some that are specifically for sport and others whose aesthetic really allows you to use them for sport as well. As out to dinner or to work, etc. And that's how I use them. If you'd like to try ROKA eyeglasses and sunglasses, you can go to roka.com, that's roka.com and enter the code HUBERMAN to save 20% off your first order. Again, that's ROKA, roka.com, and enter the code HUBERMAN at checkout.
Today's episode is also brought to us by Levels. Levels is a program that lets you see how different foods and behaviors affect your health by giving you real time feedback on your diet using a continuous glucose monitor. One of the most important factors impacting your immediate and long term health is the way that your body manages its blood glucose, or sometimes referred to as blood sugar levels. To maintain energy and focus throughout the day, you want to keep your blood glucose steady without big spikes or dips. Using Levels, you can monitor how different types of foods and different food combinations, as well as food timing and things like exercise, combine to impact your blood glucose levels.
I started using Levels a little over a year ago and it gave me a lot of insight into how specific foods were spiking my blood sugar and then leaving me feeling tired for several hours afterwards, as well as how the spacing of exercise and my meals was impacting my overall energy. And in doing so, it really allowed me to optimize how I eat, what I eat, when I exercise, and so on, such that my blood glucose levels and energy levels are stable throughout the day. If you're interested in learning more about Levels and trying a continuous glucose monitor yourself, go to levels.link/huberman. Right now, Levels is offering an additional two free months of membership. Again, that's levels.link/huberman to get two free months of membership.
We are always striving to make the Huberman Lab Podcast better, and to that end, we need your help. Over the next month we are going to be carrying out a survey. The purpose of the survey is to improve the Huberman Lab Podcast according to your feedback. We put together a brief survey to understand what you love about the podcast. Hopefully you love a few things at least, or maybe just one thing, as well as what you think could be improved, or perhaps the many things that you think could be improved about the Huberman Lab Podcast. Basically what we are asking is to get your feedback so that we can improve any and all things about the Huberman Lab Podcast. The survey does not take long and every single response will be reviewed as a thank you for completing the survey. We are offering two months free of the Huberman Lab Premium channel. If you're already a member of the Huberman Lab Premium channel, do not worry. You will get an additional two free months for carrying out this survey. You can find the link to the survey in the show notes for this podcast episode and on our website, hubermanlab.com.
So if you would be so kind as to take a few minutes to fill out the survey and help us continue with bringing you the best possible content here at the Huberman Lab Podcast. And as always, thank you for your interest in science.
And now for My discussion with Dr. David Linden.
Professor Linden, welcome.
David Linden: Thanks so much for having me.
Andrew Huberman: I've been looking forward to our conversation, and we have a lot to talk about.
David Linden: We do.
Andrew Huberman: Lots of different aspects of science, lots of different aspects of personal journey, and what you're confronting now as it relates to your health and your future. I want to start off with a question that I learned from the one and only, the great Karl Deisseroth, who was the first guest on this podcast, my colleague at Stanford. And for those of you that don't recognize Karl's name, he is a absolute phenom. He's a active clinical psychiatrist. So he's an MD. And he also is a bioengineer who's developed a lot of the modern tools for probing the brain. And anytime I've met with Karl, the first thing he says is, what are you most excited about lately?
David Linden: That's a good question.
Andrew Huberman: It is. So I'm going to steal that approach and say, what are you most excited about lately?
David Linden: Well, very, very lately. The most interesting thing that I read in neuroscience Is the answer to a scientific problem that I think is really dear to a lot of people's hearts. And that is, what are the nerve endings in the genitals that are responsible for sexual sensation? And if you think about it right, people can feel sexy from being touched on lots of different parts of the body. But there's something special about the genitals. Doesn't matter. male or female or intersex or gay or straight or bi or whatever you are, the genitals are a hotspot. And why? And you'd think as biologists, we'd know this by now, this would be something we could just answer. But It's been a mystery for a long time. And if you go back to 1860, there was a German neuroanatomist named Krause, and he cut thin sections of tissue from the penis and the clitoris, and he looked at them under the microscope and he saw a particular kind of nerve ending there that has since been called the Krause corpuscle. And there were lots of them in these two places. And so he thought, well, maybe this is the cellular basis of sexual sensation. Maybe these are the particular nerve endings that are responsible for this.
But there were some things that were in favor of that and some not. So these nerve endings are also in some other places that people can find more or less sexy, like they're in the nipples and they're in the lips and they're in the anus, all places that get popular in that domain, but they're also in places like the cornea or the lining of the joints. So distribution doesn't quite make sense. And so it was never known. And so if you wanted to really test as a scientist whether these nerve endings are responsible, you want to record their electrical signals while the genitals are being touched. You'd want to inactivate these cells and see if you could interfere with sexual sensation. And this, in a preprint from David Ginty's group at Harvard, is just what they have been able to do in mice. They found a way to label and record from and activate and inactivate artificially the Krause corpuscles.
You see a nerve ending in the skin. It could be conveying all kinds of information. It could be tuned for hot or for cold or for itch or for pain or for inflammation or for mechanical sensation, stretching, vibration, indentation. And sure enough, when they recorded from these Krause corpuscles, they really are mechanical sensors, as you would expect if they were involved in sexual sensation. So that was good. And then the other thing then what they did is they tried to artificially turn them on. And so the way they did that is they used genetic tricks to express one of Karl Deisseroth's molecule that activates neurons when blue light is shown on them. And they found that if they express this artificial protein in the Krause cells in a male mouse and then shine blue light, the mouse gets an erection. All right, so far, so good. What happens if you turn them off? Well, if you turn them off in a male mouse, it's just as interested in females when they're in heat, but it won't mount and thrust and ejaculate as much. And if you turn them off in a female mouse during the time of her cycle where she would normally be sexually receptive, you find that she is much less interested, she's much less likely to let him mount. She's much less likely to let him finish.
So this is the remarkable result that finally, after all these years, since 1860, now we know what the nerve endings are that convey sexual sensation. And like all good science, then there are a lot of questions that are really interesting to our everyday lives. Like, people like different things in bed and have different propensity for orgasm or like to be touched in different ways. Well, is part of that reason because of individual variation in their Krause corpuscle structure? We know that sexual sensation diminishes with aging. Is that in part because Krause corpuscle density is lost from the skin of the genitals? And that's a reasonable idea because we know, for example, that fine touch sensors in the fingertips, so called Merkel and Meissner endings, also named after German anatomists, like so many things, are, are also lost with age. So that's a reasonable idea.
So this finding from Ginty's lab has opened up a whole world of science. And my own lab doesn't work on touch. But I've been a fanboy of touch for many, many years, mostly because where I work at Johns Hopkins Medical School, there have been many terrific touch researchers. It's been a world center for it, and I hear about it over lunch, and I got all fired up. So years ago, I wrote a book about it, and I still follow the field. And this is the most interesting thing in that field recently.
Andrew Huberman: And as I recall, Ginty was your neighbor at Hopkins before he moved to Harvard.
David Linden: That's right. He was one of the ones. Ginty, Steven Chiu, Michael, Katerina, Xinjiang Dong. There have been a number of world leaders in the cellular basis of touch sensation at Hopkins.
Andrew Huberman: Do you recall if, in the pre print that you were describing, there was an experiment where they activated these Krause corpuscles in females?
David Linden: It's funny you should mention that. I sent that exact email to David Ginty, and they said, they are in the process of doing that right now, and they don't quite know yet. And so I asked him. I said, so, for example, is erection of the clitoris even a thing in mice? He says, well, we're really not sure. So we're activating the Krause corpuscle in female mice, and we're just kind of staring at it and looking and see if anything happens. Does the body change shape? Is there a color change? They don't even quite know what it is they're looking for because it's that much on the leading edge of things. But it's a good question.
Andrew Huberman: Or perhaps the female mice would be more willing to mate outside of the usual time frame of receptivity. If these Kraus corpuscles are stimulated, that's possible.
David Linden: My guess would be not, because I think that the hormonal regulation of receptivity is like a sledgehammer and very hard to overcome, but they might be more willing to continue mating or mate for longer during their fertile time.
Andrew Huberman: And I just want to remind people, because we had a guest recently, Dr. Rena Malik, who's a urologist, reproductive and sexual health expert. She's an MD, and she made clear that the clitoris and the penis come from the same embryonic origin. They are analogous tissues in different individuals.
I do have one more question about this sexual touch thing. These are peripheral nerves, right? So these are not of the brain and spinal cord. They are in what we call the periphery. And my understanding is that peripheral neurons regenerate and can remodel themselves extensively in ways that neurons within the brain and spinal cord tend to remodel less, especially as one gets older out of the so called critical period. Is it possible that these Krause corpuscules and their patterns of innervation within the genitals change according to the stimulation that people experience? In other words, is sexual sensation experience dependent?
David Linden: That is a great question, and so we don't know because monitoring this in people is not technically possible. It requires cadaver tissue, so you can only do it once. In animals, it will be possible, and it could be for a couple of different reasons. In other words, it could be. I think what you're imagining is that there's actual structural plasticity. If you looked at these Krause corpuscles, or that you would actually see them changing their shape or their size or their density as a result of experience. But what can also happen is a phenomena like desensitization. That is to say, when there's stimulation for a long time, then the receptors transiently can become less sensitive to touch. And it's well known, particularly in males, that chronic masturbation can produce desensitization of sexual sensation in the penis. And that could be as a result of a physical change, a morphological change in the Krause corpuscles. But it's more likely to be a change in their function that you wouldn't be able to simply see by looking at an outline of their structure in the microscope.
Andrew Huberman: Such an interesting topic. Thanks for opening things up with that. And I'll have to check out this preprint. I'm also a huge fan of David Ginty's work and colleagues. There are many people involved in that domain of work, of course.
I'd like to talk about your recent book and the sort of underlying basis of what led you to write it and what intrigued you about this idea of human individuality. The book Unique is one that we'll provide a link to in the show note captions. And it's a very interesting idea that we are all different, especially coming from a neuroscientist who we were trained, at least similarly, to learn that, sure, the bumps and ripples of the brain and the fine wiring of the brain is different, and we are all unique and different. We have different shapes, aka morphologies. But focusing on human individuality is not something that modern neuroscience or classic neuroscience has really done much of. It's really focused on how people do X or people do Y this way. Tell us about Unique and tell us about human individuality.
David Linden: Yeah, well, I mean, you're absolutely right. So when I look at the experiments in my own lab, how do we do them? Well, we work on mice. Do we work on mice with genetic variation? No, we work on highly inbred mice that are designed to be as genetically similar to each other as possible. And then we raise them basically in prison, in little cells, which may not be a good idea, and we try to give them as similar experience as possible.
Andrew Huberman: They are given toys and food and water. But I agree, it resembles a prison of sorts. They aren't free to roam.
David Linden: They have nothing like the experience of a wild mouse, let me put it that way. And, yes, as you said correctly, there are plenty of experiments where there is enrichment for mice, and they love it. So, for example, in our lab, when we put running wheels in the cages or mice and let them run overnight, they're active at night. Your average mouse will run 2 km in a night for a little tiny mouse. And some of the mice are so intense, they will run 20 km. Imagine a mouse doing 20K? But it will happen. They really, really like it. They don't like being in prison. They want to exercise, and they're really bored.
So, yes, to get back to your general point, so much of science is designed to try to find general principles of function of the brain, of physiology, of genetics, and to ignore individual variation. But individual variation is so important to our human experience, and actually is so important to the process of evolution and natural selection and how species make their way in the world, that it's something that requires a lot of attention. And to me, what's really fascinating is that when you look at the variation in the way sense organs function. It's almost a miracle that we can agree on a common reality at all, even within the human species. And this is true of some senses more than others. Obviously, in your world, in the retina, we have various kinds of loss of color vision that are well known. And some other more complicated phenomena having to do with impairments in the perception of motion or form. But the place where this really happens is in the olfactory system.
So we have approximately 400 functional receptors for different odorant molecule smells in our nose. And if you sequence the genomes of many people, you find that the DNA that encodes for these odorant receptors is unusually variable from individual to individual. As a matter of fact, if you take two different people, on average, they will have functional differences in 30% of their odor receptors. And if you do, as Leslie Vosshall and her colleagues did at Rockefeller University, and give odor tests where they give people different things to smell, and then they dilute them and find the threshold at which they can detect them, you find enormous changes from people to people, both in general terms. Some people are just better smellers than others. But in terms of individual odors as well, there are some odors that some people can't detect, and other people smell one way. For example, there's a secreted hormone called androstenone. Androstenone, there are some people who can't smell it at all. For some people, it smells like rather pleasant, like cut grass. And for some people it smells foul, like urine or sweat. And it just depends on genetic variation in one particular odorant receptor.
Andrew Huberman: Sorry to interrupt another phenomenal researcher who studies olfaction, among other things, Catherine Dulac. I once heard say that some people have a gene that for them makes the smell of microwave popcorn, they experience that smell as vomit. And other people who lack this gene, like the smell of microwave popcorn, or at least for them, it's not aversive. So it can really be a binary response.
David Linden: Well, it can, and actually, that's a very particular funny case. So the relevant chemical there is butyric acid and also isovaleric acid. And so there are researchers, I think Rachel Hertz is one of them, who have given a mixture of these two chemicals to people. And if they say, this is Parmesan cheese, they go, oh, yeah, that's Parmesan cheese. And if they give it to other people and say, this is vomit, they'll go, oh, yeah, that's vomit. And if they tell people, they give them one vial and say, this is Parmesan cheese, and they go, yeah. And they give another one. They say, it's vomit. They go, yeah. And then they say, well, actually, we fooled you. It was the same vial. They say, no, you didn't. You must have made a mistake. They're convinced that they couldn't have been the same thing.
So this points out, not only is there genetic variation that is responsible for how individuals perceive odor, but we are incredibly suggestible in terms of odors, and we are very dependent upon them in terms of cultural context. And this can be learned, and this is central to our humanity in the sense that we humans are what I like to call the anti-pandas. Pandas live in one spot in southern China, and they eat one thing, bamboo, and that's it. Humans are the opposite. Humans can live in any ecological niche in the world, from the Tropics to the Poles. And humans eat a wide, wide, wide variety of foods. And as a result, it means that we have to have a very plastic olfactory system. There have to be very few things that we find innately aversive. There are only a handful of odors. Rotting meat odors, molecules with the evocative names like cadaverine and putracine are things that even babies, when they're newborn, find aversive. But other things happen that they need to be learned. For example, pretty much every adult finds poop odors unpleasant. But babies happily play with their own poop. They have to learn that that's disgusting. It's not because babies have a different nose, it's because they have to learn culturally, not innate. It is not innate. There are only a few innate odor aversions and a few innate taste aversions that we're born with. And the other things are elaborated culturally. And we can think about this in terms of how we talk about odors. So, for example, we might say vanilla smells sweet. Well, that's weird. Those are two different senses. How can something smell sweet? That's like saying it sounds red, right?
Andrew Huberman: It's a statement about synesthesia.
David Linden: Right? But how did it come to pass? And do people say that vanilla smells sweet everywhere in the world? Well, the answer is no. So in places in the world where vanilla is used with sugar, in sweet foods, like desserts, then people say that vanilla smells sweet, or mint similarly smells sweet if it is typically used together with sugar. But if you go to a place like Vietnam, where mint is mostly used in savory dishes, people won't say that mint smells sweet.
Andrew Huberman: So there's a paired association there that, at least at our level of conscious understanding feeds back onto what we call olfactory or smell perception. But really, it must be a paired association at some point in development.
David Linden: Yeah, it is. It absolutely is a paired association. And it's something that goes on continually through your life, right? I mean, lots of people, for example, have stories of foods that they wouldn't eat as a child, but they came to like as an adult. A good example of that is coffee. A lot of people have to overcome bitter aversion to become coffee aficionados. So this feeds into the more general theme that there is no pure perception. Perception is inference. It's not like there is a purely objective world that can somehow make its way through the senses, and we can perceive that as the truth. All of our perception through all of our senses, both the outward pointing senses at the world, like smell and taste and sight and hearing, and the inward pointing senses like balance and is my stomach full and things like that. All of them are based on experience and expectation and the situation of the moment.
Andrew Huberman: As many of you know, I've been taking AG1 one daily since 2012, so I'm delighted that they're sponsoring the podcast. AG1 is a vitamin mineral probiotic drink that's designed to meet all of your foundational nutrition needs. Now, of course, I try to get enough servings of vitamins and minerals through whole food sources that include vegetables and fruits every day, but oftentimes I simply can't get enough servings. But with AG1, I'm sure to get enough vitamins and minerals and the probiotics that I need. And it also contains adaptogens to help buffer stress.
Simply put, I always feel better when I take AG1. I have more focus and energy and I sleep better. And it also happens to taste great. For all these reasons, whenever I'm asked if you could take just one supplement, what would it be? I answer AG1.
If you'd like to try AG1, go to drinkag1.com/huberman to claim a special offer. They'll give you five free travel packs plus a year supply of vitamin D3 K2. Again, that's drinkag1.com/huberman.
Are there any examples of uniqueness of visual perception that come to mind? I recently did a social media post that involved it was essentially three rings, a blue ring, a red ring, and a blue ring in the center. Or perhaps it was the other way around. Excuse me, it was red, blue red, and I asked which ring is in front? Or are they all in the same plane? Now of course, it's a two dimensional image and interestingly, it splits out into about thirds. Some people see the blue ring in front quite a bit, others see a red ring out in front, others see them all in the same plane. And this, we think, has to do with differences in two things between individuals. One is the distribution of the cone photoreceptors, which we know is essentially random between individuals, maybe even between the two eyes. And that gives rise to this phenomenon of chromatic aberration, which is the displacement of the visual image according to the wavelength of the light. And we won't get into the physics of it now. I'll soon do a post that hopefully distills it in a manner that's simple enough that people understand, but clearly, some people see certain colors in front of others, and the person right next to them could see the opposite color in front. And others say, what are you talking about? All the colors are in exactly the same plane of vision. So that's the one that I know. I'm guessing you know some others and perhaps some more robust ones.
David Linden: Well, I think perhaps this is maybe not what you had in mind, but one way in which experience modifies the visual world has to do with how much light you're exposed to in the first five years or so of your life. And so kids that don't get outside are much more likely to be myopic, and it actually is--
Andrew Huberman: Near sighted.
David Linden: Near sighted, yes, when they grow up, than kids who got outside. And we now know that at least part of the story is that light seems to stimulate the expression of a class of molecules called trophic factors that you're well acquainted with that actually change the shape of the eyeball. So it's not really the structure of the retina or the lens of the cornea. The actual degree of elongation of the eyeball changes, changing the way the retina sits relative to the lens. And that seems to be light dependent early in life, which gives rise to a higher incidence of myopia. To me, this is really. Well, first of all, it's news you can use. You should get your kids outside for all kinds of reasons.
Andrew Huberman: And you should get outside too, especially in the morning. Set that circadian rhythm.
David Linden: I know that's a famous Hubermanesque point.
Andrew Huberman: They're going to be putting me in the grave, David, and I'm going to be telling people to. Or maybe it'll be on my tombstone. He'll say, get sunlight in your eyes, especially on cloudy days, because there is still sunlight, even if you can't see the physical object of the sun on cloudy days?
David Linden: Well, I think this whole idea of having traits that are dependent upon early life experience is fascinating because there are a number of situations where you would guess that something is genetic, but it isn't. It's actually dependent on early life experience. And there's an amazing story about this having to do with the early days of World War II. So in the early days of World War II, the Japanese army just swept through Asia. They defeated the British and Malaysia and Singapore, they overrun Thailand and Burma, and they were knocking on the gates of India, and everything was going great, except the Japanese army had a problem. There were an enormous number of their soldiers who became incapacitated with heat stroke. Their core temperature got too hot. And when the army doctors examined them, they found this was much more likely to happen in soldiers who came from the northern part of Japan, Hokkaido, where it snows in the winter, as opposed to the southern part of Japan, like Kyushu, which is a semi tropical environment. And the classical explanation, the biologists, like us would guess, would say, oh, all right, well, this has happened genetically over many years. You have a family that's been in Kyushu for many generations, and you've selected four gene variants that allow you to tolerate the heat better. And we know actually what this is.
So if you're more heat tolerant, it's because you have more of a particular class of sweat gland called the eccrine sweat glands, the sort of saltwater sweat glands, not the lipidy, stinky armpit sweat glands called the apricrine ones, the eccrine ones, you have a higher fraction of them that are innervated, meaning that the signals from your brain that say your core is too hot can then make you sweat. So the total density of sweat glands between northern and southern soldiers in Japan wasn't different, but the southern soldiers tend to had a higher degree of innervation. All right, so we said, okay, well, this happened genetically over many generations, but if you look at those rare cases where you had soldiers from a long established northern family and their parents moved south, and then they grew up in the southern location, they had high sweat gland innervation. They were, well, heat tolerant. Conversely, if you had a well established southern Kyushu family and they moved to Hokaido and then had their child, that child developed the northern sweat gland innervation pattern.
Andrew Huberman: So, meaning less nerve innervation of those sweat glands, as you mentioned before, just as many sweat glands, just less nerve innervation. Therefore, those sweat glands could not be activated. They couldn't dump heat as well. Their heat tolerance was lower. Correct?
David Linden: Exactly right. And what's wonderful is that this gives an advantage that you can't get through evolution and that it can happen right away in one generation. Evolutionary change is slow, and you can adapt as a species and as a family over many, many generations. But when you have a phenomenon that is set by early life experience, well, then you can benefit from that early life experience within your own life. It's not that your great, great great grandchildren will ultimately benefit you benefit.
Another wonderful example of this comes from field mice, voles. And we were talking earlier about how we both worked with the scientist Irving Zucker at Berkeley, who was a specialist in voles. And what people found is that if you take wild caught field mice and you have pregnant mothers and you have them in the lab, but you manipulate the lights so that you have artificial spring. In other words, day length is getting longer day after day during the pregnancy, then what happens is when their pups are born, they will have a low density of fur, anticipating summer temperatures.
If you, however, put them in artificial fall, where day length is getting shorter, they will be born now with high density of fur, anticipating winter temperatures. And of course, you can do this no matter what the season actually is in the world by manipulating these lights in the lab. And so, like the sweating Japanese soldiers, this is a great example of early life plasticity and just the sort of trait that, if you ask someone, they would probably guess is heritable, but actually is not.
Andrew Huberman: Thanks for mentioning Irving Zucker, who, as you also mentioned, was an advisor to us both, who's done incredible work in circadian biology, seasonal rhythms, hormones and behavior. I have such reverence for Irving. The experiment you mentioned made me smile wide because it's but one of, gosh, maybe hundreds of incredible studies. So if people are interested in seasonal rhythms and circadian rhythms and biology of the most interesting kind, definitely check out Irving Zucker's work at Berkeley. I'll provide a link to his PubMed there.
Since we've been taking a tour of individual variation in olfactory perception, visual perception, and now heat tolerance, I have to ask, are you aware of any examples off the top of your head in the auditory domain that particularly intrigue you?
David Linden: Yeah. Well, I would say one really interesting example has to do with perfect pitch. So perfect pitch as a trait, that is to say, you have the ability to hear a note played and say, oh, that's a C sharp, right? This is a pretty rare trait. So even if you look among highly trained musicians, if you went to Peabody Conservatory at my university, Johns Hopkins, and tested people there, you would find a higher incidence of perfect pitch than you would in the general population. But still, maybe one in ten trained musicians have perfect pitch. And parenthetically, having perfect pitch doesn't necessarily make you a better musician, but it's an interesting phenomena. And so the question is, well, is perfect pitch heritable? And the answer is, when you look at twin studies, which is what we use to estimate heritability, the answer is, it's kind of low. There is a heritable component, but it accounts for, my recollection is on the order of 30 40% of the variability in perfect pitch. However, if people receive ear training starting at a young age, the chance that they will develop perfect pitch can improve drastically.
Andrew Huberman: In your book, Unique, do you cover aspects of human individuality that extend beyond the perception domain into the cognitive domain?
David Linden: Well, yeah, absolutely. And I think it's good to set the stage here if we're going to be talking about heritability and human individuality. If I can go off on a little bit of a riff for the benefit of your listeners and viewers here.
Andrew Huberman: Please.
David Linden: So, if you look at human traits, whether they are behavioral traits like shyness, or very straightforward morphological traits like height, what you tend to find is that there are very few traits that are entirely heritable, where all their variability can be predicted based on the gene variance you get from your mother and father. And there are a few traits that are absolutely unheritable, but that most fall in between.
So let me give an example. Everyone in the world has either wet or dry earwax, and it turns out that this is determined by variation in a single gene. The name of the gene is boring. It's ABCC11, it's an ion transporter. And there is a variation in this gene gives rise to either wet or dry earwax. It doesn't matter how your parents raised you, it doesn't matter what foods you ate growing up, doesn't matter what diseases your mother had when you were in the womb, it's 100% heritable. Well, does this mean that ABCC11, we should call it the earwax type gene? Well, no, because it's not there just for that. Like, this gene is expressed in cells in all parts of the body, doing all kinds of things. Earwax is just something that we notice. Genes don't code for traits, they code for proteins, and so we have to be careful about how we refer to them in that way. For example, the wet earwax variant of the ABCC11 gene also confers a slightly higher risk for breast cancer.
So, clearly, it's not just for earwax. It's for a bunch of things, most of which we don't yet know about. But in the case of earwax, this trait is 100% heritable. At the other end of the scale, speech accent is 0% heritable. It is entirely dependent upon the speech that you experience in your childhood. And interestingly, it's the speech of your peers more than the speech of your family, which is why the children of immigrants sound like the place where they wound up, not like their parents. And there is no evidence for any degree of heritability. Now, just to be clear, I'm talking about speech accent. Like whether you have a high or a low voice or it's nasal, are more or less resonant. These are physical things having to do with the vocal tract, and they are, in part, heritable.
Okay, so we've got one thing that's 100% heritable and one thing that's 0% heritable. But where do most things fall? Most things fall in the middle. One of the most heritable traits that we know about in humans is height. And in the United States, height is about 85% heritable. 85% of the variation in the trait of height can be explained by what you inherit from your mother and your father. Well, what's the rest? Well, it's nutrition. It's the diseases you fought off. It's also random variation, which we'll talk about a lot later.
Now, you might say, okay, well, that's an estimate for people in the US. Is this true all over the world? Well, no. If you go to a place where people routinely don't get enough nutrition and are routinely fighting off infectious diseases like this has been studied in rural Bolivia, for example, or rural India. Now, height is no longer 85% heritable, it's only 50% heritable. Why? Because people in these situations where they don't get enough nourishment, where they're fighting off these diseases, can't live up to their genetic potential for height.
If you want to make things better for the people of the world, then everyone needs to have basic things like the ability to learn and enough nutrition and decent medical care and schools in order to fulfill their genetic potential for positive traits. And height, I've used as an example, because it is very uncontroversial. But we could apply the very same analysis to intelligence, general intelligence. Now, there are people who argue about, do things like IQ tests really measure anything real. And there's been a lot of fighting in the scientific literature about this. But I think intelligence tests aren't perfect, and they are sometimes culture bound, but they are actually quite predictive of later success, and much more so than, say, SAT tests or GRE tests or MCATs or other standardized tests.
Andrew Huberman: But presumably those correlate in some way.
David Linden: They do. But the IQ tests are better actually.
Andrew Huberman: They're talking about the classic IQ test?
David Linden: I am talking about the modern variants of the classic IQ test that are administered by trained psychologists and aren't just a paper form. And so they're not perfect, and no test will be perfect, but they're pretty good. And so then, if you ask the question, well, what is the heritability for IQ test score? Well, the answer tends to be different depending upon the population. If you look, again, in countries like the US or in Western Europe that are fairly affluent, where people tend to have good access to nutrition and medical care and schooling and kids get to play and they're not traumatized by war, then IQ test score is heritable in the ballpark of 60, 70%.
But if you look at people who don't have those benefits, who are poor, and this can be in the United States as well, if you look at communities that face discrimination and have consistently poor health care in schools, then IQ is less heritable. Why? For the very same reason that it is in height. Because people can't live up to their genetic potential when they don't have the basic things that everybody needs.
Andrew Huberman: So presumably, if two identical twins, and I realize they aren't identical, but you're familiar with twins, you have twin children. If two identical twins are raised separately, the correlation in their IQ, is it that only 60? I think you said about 66% of their IQ can be predicted on the basis of their genetic makeup alone. I mean, it makes perfect sense to me as to why, if one of those twins went to schools that were demanding of a lot of different topic matter and the other one went to schools where the instruction level was really deficient, that one would perform far less well on an IQ test. Unless, of course, the IQ test isn't tapping into school based knowledge, it's tapping into some other thermometer of so called intelligence or IQ.
David Linden: Well, the thing is that good schools correlate with many other things, right? So the students that go to good schools aren't just benefiting from good schools. They tend to also have good medical care and safer, less traumatizing neighborhoods, and they're more likely to have books, parents with books in the home, and a whole number of things that are all beneficial. So when you try to do epidemiology on this, you have to be aware that things are very deeply interconnected, but you bring up a good point.
So it turns out that the way we get these estimates of heritability, there's two ways. One way is to compare so called identical or monozygotic twins with so called fraternal or dizygotic twins. So the identical twins will share nearly 100% of their gene variance, and on average, fraternal twins share 50% of their gene variance. And generally speaking, when people do these studies in order to avoid confounds of sex, they'll compare same sex fraternal twins. So boys to boys and girls to girls. And when you put these incidents into a formula called Fisher's equation, then you can come up with an estimate of the heritability of the trait. But there is an assumption present in that, and it's called the equal environment assumption. You're saying, well, two kids raised in the same family have the same environment. Well, that's not always true. Right? That can be violated by a number of different situations.
So it turns out that a more powerful but much more difficult way to estimate heritability is by looking at twins reared apart, either identical twins or fraternal twins reared apart. And there was a landmark study called the The Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart, which is abbreviated MISTRA, that is really the gold standard for assessing the heritability of many different human traits, both behavioral traits, but also disease incidents. But, of course, it's a Small-N, because the population of identical twins reared apart that you can get into the lab isn't that large. They had something. I don't remember the exact numbers, but they had something like 80 some identicals and 50 some fraternals in their sample. But by doing this, they were able to come up with a lot of interesting estimates. And so, for example, most personality traits, what the psychologists use the acronym OCEAN to mean, Openness, Conscientiousness, Empathy, Agreeableness, and Neuroticism. I think I got that right. David Linden : [LAUGHING]
Andrew Huberman: Spelled right.
David Linden: That these traits, on average, tend to be about 50% heritable. And so, okay, you say, all right, well, 50% of those personality traits is heritable. The rest has got to be like how you were raised. It's got to be in your family. And so everyone was shocked when they actually did the analysis and found that family has almost nothing to do with it. What? Are you kidding? It's got to. And I think the important thing to realize is these traits I just listed, we call these personality traits, but they are not the sum total of the way you are in the world. Parents can inculcate many things in their children. They can demonstrate trades, so people are much more likely to go into an occupation if their parents did. They can inculcate moral ideas and religious ideas, but in terms of these OCEAN personality traits, they have astonishingly little to do with it.
So then this brings up the question, well, if 50% of the variation in these personality traits is not from your genetics and it's not from your family, where does it come from? And the answer seems to be, is that it comes from the random nature of the development of the body and the nervous system. And this is a point that I think many people don't understand. This is something that biologists know, but we've done a very poor job of communicating to the general public.
The genome, all your DNA, all 3 billion bases of DNA, all 19,000 or so genes in a human don't make a blueprint for making your body and brain. It's not a schematic diagram that connects everything to everything, particularly in the nervous system, where we have these hundreds of trillions of connections. Rather, it's a rather vague recipe. So the genome doesn't say, oh, okay, you glutamate using neuron in the brain region called the thalamus, grow for 200 microns towards the top, and then cross the midline, and then grow towards the ear for another distance. No. It says something like, hey, you, bunch of glutamate neurons in the thalamus over here in this area, about half of you cross the midline. And so what does this mean in terms of individual variation? Well, for some individuals, 40% of their axons will cross the midline of the brain, and for another individual, 60% will, even in identical twins, and as you correctly said a moment ago, identical twins aren't really identical, either in their bodies or their temperament. So if you take newborn identical twins and you give them a CT scan just to measure the shape of their organs, they're not the same. You might have one twin whose spleen is 30% larger than the other twins, or whose liver is 30% smaller than the other twins, Even though they have the exact same DNA, and they were lying right next to each other in the womb and presumably had the same or very similar fetal environment. And the reason is the random, or, as we say, stochastic nature of neural development. A great way to study this is with nine-banded armadillo. I know we're getting weird here, but.
Andrew Huberman: I love the armadillo because I've been told. Tell me, I don't want to interrupt you too long, but as far as I know, the only animal in North America that carries leprosy?
David Linden: That is true.
Andrew Huberman: And there's a lot of twinning going on in armadillos, right?
David Linden: Well, what there is is actually quadding. So armadillos, or the nine-banded armadillo in particular, and there are different armadillos. I'm not really an armadillo specialist, so I don't know if this holds for all of them. But the nine-banded armadillo is born as identical quadruplets.
Andrew Huberman: Awesome.
David Linden: So you can take these identical quadruplet, newly born armadillo. I don't know. You think you call them pups? I don't know what a baby armadillo is called. I'm sure there's some particular word for it.
Andrew Huberman: Someone will tell us, I'm sure someone in the comments on YouTube. What is the name of a baby armadillo? I know, like a ferret. Baby ferrets are kits. The moms are jills, the dads are bobs. I used to be obsessed with this kind of naming. It's a business of ferrets or what is like a gang of raccoons or whatever. So if you can tell us what the name is for the baby armadillos as well as what do you call a group of armadillos, you win the pride associated with being right.
David Linden: That's right. Right. One of my favorites is an ostentation of peacocks.
Andrew Huberman: Amazing.
David Linden: Or a murder of crows.
Andrew Huberman: Who comes up with this stuff?
David Linden: I know.
Andrew Huberman: Or a raft of otters.
David Linden: David Linden : [LAUGHING]
Andrew Huberman: I think that's correct.
David Linden: So if you have four newborn identical nine-banded armadillos, then this is a great model system that biologists can use to study stochastic differences in development. And sure enough, their brains are wired slightly differently, their bodies are slightly different. If you test them behaviorally, even very, very early in life, they have different propensities. Some are bolder and will explore more. Some will tend to hide in the corner more. And we know this from the lab. You get a box of mice that are inbred from the breeder and you pluck them out. And they're not behaviorally identical. Some might try to bite your hand, some will run away, some will stand stock still. Where does this behavioral variation come from in mice that are nearly genetically identical? Well, it comes from a bunch of things. They don't always have exactly equal experience, but mostly it comes from the pseudo random, stochastic nature of development.
Andrew Huberman: I'd like to take a quick break and acknowledge our sponsor, InsideTracker. InsideTracker is a personalized nutrition platform that analyzes data from your blood and DNA to help you better understand your body and help you meet your health goals. I'm a big believer in getting regular blood work done for the simple reason that many of the factors that impact your immediate and long term health can only be analyzed from a quality blood test. However, with a lot of blood tests out there, you get information back about blood lipids, about hormones and so on, but you don't know what to do with that information. With InsideTracker, they have a personalized platform that makes it very easy to understand your data, that is, to understand what those lipids, what those hormone levels, etc, mean, and behavioral supplement, nutrition and other protocols to adjust those numbers to bring them into the ranges that are ideal for your immediate and long term health. InsideTracker's ultimate plan now includes measures of both Apo B and of insulin, which are key indicators of cardiovascular health and energy regulation.
If you'd like to try InsideTracker, you can visit insidetracker.com/huberman to get 20% off any of InsideTracker's plans. Again, that's insidetracker.com/huberman to get 20% off.
And is the pseudorandom, stochastic nature of development one of the major, excuse me, driving forces for evolution? Because we hear about mutations and we always think, or people tend to think, rather, that mutations are bad. But of course, mutations provide the variation that can also subserve adaptive traits. I mean, if you're a fan of the X-Men, as I am huge fan of the X-Men, the entire series, every single one, including the Wolverine movies, you quickly come to learn that genetic mutation is at the heart of variation, which is at the heart of individuality, which is what we're talking about, right?
David Linden: And so genetic variation is at the heart of individuality. But there are also these other things, right, that we've talked about. There's the effects of early life experience, and there is the stochastic nature of development. Because if you, through the randomness of development, happen to have, like, a particularly great liver, you're not going to pass that on to your children, right? That isn't in your germline. You won't pass that trait along.
Andrew Huberman: Just a brief insert here on germline. We had Oded Rechavi on the podcast who studies epigenetic transmission and inheritance. It's not Lamarckian, we have to point that out. But inheritance of sort of acquired traits, it does happen. The germline is the genes that are present in the sperm and in the eggs. All the other cells of your body have genes, of course, but the best way to put this is simply going to the gym and getting fit does not make your children more fit, because the germ line, as far as we know, is not modified in a direct way. In other words, the DNA within sperm and eggs are not modified according to your behaviors in most, but not all cases.
David Linden: That's right. And as you correctly said about Oded's work and other people's work, there is what's called transgenerational epigenetic inheritance, which means that you can have traits that are passed not from one generation to the next, but even two generations to the grandchildren, that don't require modification of DNA. But to date, that has been shown very convincingly in worms and in plants. The evidence in mammals is really not there yet, in my opinion, and most of the claims for that. And it's a very popular thing to say, I epigenetically inherited my grandmother's or great grandmother's trauma. The evidence at present is poor. Actually, a lot of it comes from epidemiology, most of which came from famines in the urchalics region of northern Sweden. And they had very good medical records. And they said, oh, well, if your grandfather went through the famine, then you're more likely to have this trait. If you're male, or if your grandmother went through this, then if you're male, have this trait and your female have that trait. And, I mean, there are two problems. One is that there's not a biological mechanism, but the other problem is that the way these things were discovered is by something called harking or hypothesizing after the results are known.
They did very many statistical comparisons to try to find something significant. And you know from your work in the lab that when you do many comparisons, you're going to get some things that look significant just occasionally or randomly through luck. And you have to apply a statistical correction called a Bonferroni correction when you make many, particularly post hoc comparisons after the experiment, comparisons to set the bar much higher for accepting that data. And most of those studies, they didn't apply that correction. And I remain unconvinced of transgenerational epigenetic inheritance in mammals.
Now, let's be clear. Just because it hasn't been shown convincingly now, doesn't mean that it won't be. There are some good people working very hard on this, and they may well describe a mechanism and show this convincingly in the years to come. But right now, you may well inherit your grandma's or great grandma's trauma, but you're probably doing it socially, not through marks on your DNA that changes how your genes are expressed or not expressed.
Andrew Huberman: Yes. So I subscribe to the idea that there is absolutely, certainly transgenerational inheritance of parenting and upbringing. Right? I mean, your grandparents raise your parents who raise you. Not always, people can be adopted. In fact, I have adopted members of my family. But if I understand what you're saying correctly, the evidence that, for instance, some stress related gene was modified during a trauma in my grandparents or great grandparents, and the idea that that was passed to me through my parents, that the evidence there is far weaker.
David Linden: Right. And when you think about it, well, how did that happen? That had to get into your grandparents sperm or egg cell and then produce that effect in the brain of your parent, and then it had to get into their sperm or egg cell and then contribute to producing it in you.
Andrew Huberman: But fragmentation of DNA in sperm or in eggs is a common thing, especially as people age. DNA, sperm and eggs fragment. And it's possible that some of those mutations still allow for viable embryos. So in theory, the germline could be changed by environmental events.
David Linden: Well, right. But now I think you're starting to talk about things that are heritable. Right? You're not talking about marks on DNA. You're talking about the structure of the DNA itself. And that is its own separate issue. Now, I think I want to be really careful about this, because what is now, I think, fairly well established is that you can transfer things epigenetically over a single generation if, as a result of experiences that the mother has during pregnancy.
So, for example, we know that during the 1918 pandemic flu, many women were pregnant and got the flu. And if you look at their children, you find interesting statistical anomalies. And those children, for example, the males, wound up going into the army for World War II. And of course, the army does a complete physical, and the records are very good. So you can go into that database and you find that the male children that were in utero during the winter of 1918, during the pandemic flu, are on average, a millimeter or two shorter, you might say a millimeter or two. That's nothing. But in a huge statistical sample of millions of people, that's enormously significant. More interesting is that the incidence of schizophrenia went up about fourfold, from about 1% to about 4%. And even though autism wasn't a term in 1918 yet, I don't think it came along later. What we now retrospectively would call autism also went up by about fourfold.
So there's something about mom being stressed and carrying the fetus at a particular stage that seems to impact brain development in a way that then makes that child more likely to be schizophrenic or autistic when they grow up.
Andrew Huberman: Do we know that it's stress? And my recollection of this, I believe this was the late Paul Sternberg's work as well. I maybe have that name incorrect, but in any event, that it is, if pregnant mom gets the flu in the first trimester, you see this higher incidence of schizophrenia and autistic offspring. But do we know that it's stress per se, because it's stressful to have the flu, but the flu is a bunch of other things. Could be fever, could be some breakdown in the immune barrier. I just want to open up the number of variables that this could be. Or do we know that it's something in the hypothalamic–pituitary, so called stress axis, that is adrenals? So hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis. Is it elevated cortisol, or could it literally be an immune neural interaction of some other sort?
David Linden: It's probably the last thing you mentioned, an immune neural interaction. And the reason I say that is that Gloria Choi at MIT, together with her collaborators, has made a mouse model of this phenomena. So she takes pregnant female mice, and she injects them with something that she doesn't actually infect them with virus. She puts a chemical in that is on the coat of viruses that mimics viral infection. And then what happens is that, in a way that, interestingly, is an interaction with the bacterial content of her gut produces a surge of an immune signaling molecule called interleukin-17.
Interleukin-17 can pass through the placenta into the fetus, and if it's present, just as you said, at a particular point in development, doesn't work anywhere during pregnancy, but during something that is sort of the mouse equivalent of the first trimester, if that occurs, it causes disordered development of the layers of the cortex. Instead of it looking like layers of a cake, you see balls and clumps of cells. And parenthetically, in some, but not all, post mortem tissue from autistic people, you can also see those balls and clumps of cells.
Andrew Huberman: Are those balls and clumps of cells thought to reflect alterations in cell migration?
David Linden: They are, yes. I don't know if it's entirely known how much of it is cell division or migration, but certainly migration is a part of it. And it's very likely that that critical moment to disrupt this ordering of the brain and produce these increases in schizophrenia or autism vulnerability are coming at a point where neurons are migrating during development. And so what Choi's group did is they did all the things you would want to do as a biologist. So they gave things to block the function of this interleukin signaling molecule, and it blocked the phenomenon. They artificially injected the signaling molecule into fetal brain when the mom hadn't been stressed, and they could reproduce it.
So I'm not saying that there aren't effects of stress hormones from the hypothalamic-pituitary axis that are important that you mentioned, but in this mouse model system work, it seems that you can produce it through this immune signaling pathway. So then the question is, well, are these mice autistic? Well, how do you know if a mouse is autistic? And the answer is, it's actually a little vague, right? There are behaviors that neuroscientists say are analogous of human autism, and one of them is, if you give a mouse a marble in its home cage, it will bury it over and over again compulsively, or bury many marbles. And people say that that is somehow analogous to some of the compulsive behaviors you see in autism. It's a bit of a stretch, right? I mean, it's a challenge to interpret mouse behavior in human terms, but it's a reasonable first step.
Andrew Huberman: Incredible. And I hope more will continue to be done as it surrounds the first trimester influenza hypothesis, because it's been around a while, and obviously there's a spectrum of what we call autism, Asperger's and nowadays people refer to it as sometimes as neuroatypical. There's some high functioning people with autism. There's some low functioning people with autism, and for that matter, there's some high functioning and low functioning people who don't have autism. But it is something that I think demands our attention and that hopefully will be resolved at some point, because also influenza is but one immune insult. And presumably pregnant women are being bombarded with all sorts of viruses and bacteria and fungal infections and fighting them off, or not fighting them off. And who knows what the variation in neuroimmune interactions exist in there that give rise to good variation, and let's call it debilitating variation.
David Linden: Well, that's absolutely right. And so one example is that we don't know what the effects are on the children who were in utero while their mothers were fighting off COVID. We won't know for a while, or the common cold, and there might be nothing, or there might be something serious lurking there, like there was for pandemic flu. And it'll be very interesting and important to find out.
Andrew Huberman: Agreed. I'd love to talk with you about mind-body, but before we do that, I would be totally remiss if I didn't ask for your broad top contour understanding of the mini brain, the cerebellum. The so called mini brain. And here's why. I've been a practicing neuroscientist for close to three decades. I know where the cerebellum is. I've dissected a bunch of them. I could tell you where a few things are in there, and I certainly have read about what the cerebellum does. But whenever I do a PubMed search on cerebellum, I see an ever expanding set of things that the cerebellum is implicated in. Not just balance, as most people hear, but also timing, also cognition. I hear about timing, in particular of motor behavior. But then I also hear that it's involved in learning and not just motor learning, and it certainly is involved in motor learning.
Perhaps that little mini brain is doing 50 or 1000 different things. But how should we think about the cerebellum? What is it doing? And what are some of its core operations that inform both what it's doing and perhaps what other areas of the brain are doing as well, because I can point to the retinas or to auditory cortex or to the thalamus. And yes, there's some mysterious nuclei in the brain, but to me, the cerebellum is one of the most cryptic and complicated structures to understand. And I know you spent some time in there, so what's the cerebellum do?
David Linden: Well, cerebellar researchers like to joke that the cerebellum is a counterweight to keep your head from falling forward.
Andrew Huberman: Oh, perfect. Well, with all the texting nowadays, people need bigger cerebellum.
David Linden: That's right. Probably over many generations. Andrew Huberman : [LAUGHING]. Then that will happen along with an expansion of the thumbs. But of course, everybody's going to text with their mind in about another ten years. Right. You'll have Elon's implant and you won't need your thumbs at all.
Andrew Huberman: Yeah. Or maybe just five. Or my friend Edward Chang, who's a neurosurgeon and works on the auditory system. He's been on this podcast before. He was saying that in theory and actually in practice, you could just record the neural output to the muscles of the speech system, essentially, and you could just amplify that and you could text without actually speaking. In fact, when we read, he told me, we are actually receiving the signals as if we were going to speak the words we're reading, but they don't quite arrive at the place where you could get a full blown postsynaptic potential. So you're not actually moving the vocal machinery. So that means the motor signals are getting sent out there, and so you're speaking what you are reading, but you just don't know it.
David Linden: That's right. And it's very analogous to what happens during the REM phase of sleep. When you have commands, your brain is issuing commands to your muscles to do things like behave in your dreams, to run away or go here or go there. But those signals actually are blocked in the brainstem, prevented from reaching your muscles, because the nerves that don't go through your brainstem, like the ones that control your eye movements, aren't subject to that blockade. That's why you can produce the rapid eye movements in REM sleep.
But, yeah, this is a general theme in the brain. A lot of times, you have the output, but then you shut it down. And there are REM sleep behavior disorders where people thrash and move in their sleep during REM, and it's because this outflow that's normally blocked isn't blocked.
Andrew Huberman: Have you ever had the reverse happen? I have. Where you wake up and you're still in so called REM atonia. You're still paralyzed, and there's that split second that feels like eternity, where you are wide awake and you cannot move. And I'll tell you, it's terrifying.
David Linden: Yeah, it's sleep paralysis and know it's been known forever. You can actually find ancient Greek depictions of people lying with a demon on their chest, paralyzing them, and that is actually from sleep paralysis. Later, Hogarth did a drawing of exactly that. So, yes, this is a well known phenomenon.
But to get back to the cerebellum, as we started. So, the cerebellum is, as you said, definitely involved in motor coordination. So, people who have damage to the cerebellum aren't paralyzed, but they tend to be clumsy. They tend to not coordinate their movements. They have a disturbed gait. If they're reaching for an object, they often overshoot it and have to make successive approximating motions to get back to their target. So that's well understood. But if you look through evolution, the cerebellum is connected to this brain region called the thalamus, and it's connected from there to many regions, including the frontal cortex, where phenomena like planning and decision making and moral sense and many aspects of personality seem to be encoded. So then the question becomes, well, that's very far away from being clumsy. What are these connections doing? And as time has gone on, I've been in this business for over 40 years now. I'm an old guy. And so initially we said, oh, yeah, cerebellum movement control, motor coordination, that's what it's for. As time goes on, as you correctly said, the cerebellum has been implicated in more and more functions, many of which are far removed from the motor system.
And if we're looking for a theme about what the cerebellum does, is that it is there to predict the immediate future. It's trying to determine what's going to happen in the next second or two to best guide behavior. And as you can imagine, this kind of general computation could be applied very well. You can see why it's important for motor systems and doing sports, and if you're trying to hit a baseball and looking at what the pitcher is doing and trying to anticipate what the pitch is, but it also comes up in a social realm. If we're trying to read someone and predict what they are going to do, is this person friend or foe? Which is one of the first things that we try to assess when meeting someone. Are they competent? Which is the second thing we try to assess when meeting something. A lot of this depends upon predictive circuitry, and it depends in part on the cerebellum, and it seems to be at least partially impaired in people who sustain cerebellar damage.
So it seems as if, interestingly, the cerebellum started out for prediction related for motor control, and through evolution, its basic computation has been applied to other non motor behaviors. Now, I'm speaking in generalities, and a lot of the details of this remain to be worked out and understood, but I would say that is, in a nutshell, the modern conception of the cerebellum.
Andrew Huberman: Thank you. Finally, somebody explains to me at a top contour but highly informed way, what the cerebellum does. I couldn't be more grateful.
David Linden: My pleasure.
Andrew Huberman: In all aspects of biology and life, the term nature versus nurture is relevant, but never so much as when thinking about the nervous system. And I know this firsthand because I've studied neural development, both the nature side, the so called hardwired stuff that genes just set up, neurons wire up to that neuron, etc. The cerebellum is in the back, the eyes are in the front, the hardwired stuff, and then the soft wired stuff, the nurture stuff, is the stuff that can be modified by experience. What are your thoughts on nature versus nurture, and should there even be a versus in there?
David Linden: Yeah, I don't think there should. And I have a lot of problems with nature versus nurture as an expression. It was popularized by Francis Galton in the 19th century, a colleague of Darwin's. And I think it's wrong or misleading in a lot of ways. So, of course, the nature in nature versus nurture is meant, in this case, to mean inheritability, right? What you inherit in the gene variations from your mother and father. And nurture means like how your parents or your community raised you. The problem I have with nurture is that it is too narrow a term that really, it should be replaced with the word experience, and experience in the broadest possible sense. Not just social experience, but the foods your mother ate when she was carrying you in utero, the diseases you fought off or your mother fought off while she was carrying you in utero, the bacterial population of your gut. So experience, meaning anything that impinges on you starting from the earliest stages of fetal development, continuing to the last day of your life, I think it should be very expansive, much, much more than social experience in the family or the community.
And as you mentioned, I have a problem with versus, because there's this idea that these things are essentially in opposition. Well, is he that way because of his gene variance, or is that way because of what happened to him? And I think the thing to realize is that experience and heredity interact in all kinds of interesting ways, some of which are oppositional and some of which are reinforcing. A classic one from genetics, has to do with a genetic disease called Phenylketonuria, or PKU, which is an inability to metabolize the dietary amino acid phenylalanine. And so in order to have this, you have to inherit broken copies of this gene from both your mother and your father. So it's a so called recessive trait. And here's where the experience comes in. It only matters if you eat foods rich in phenylalanine. If you don't, it doesn't matter that you inherited these things, right?
So that's a way in which genes and experience interact, an idea of ways in which they interact positively. Think about athletic ability. Right? So a lot of athletic ability has a heritable component. If you are born fast, for example, then you're more likely to do sports and practice them and get better sports as a result of your experience. So here, genes and experience are feeding back on each other in a positive feedback loop. So there really isn't a versus at all.
And then, of course, the last thing is that this isn't the entirety. So we talked earlier about the pseudo random nature of development, stochastic development. And so if I were to take the phrase nature versus nurture and reconfigure it, I would change it to read heritability, interacting with experience filtered through the random nature of development. Now, that doesn't fall off the tongue as elegantly as nature versus nurture. Nature versus nurture is like, if the gloves don't fit, you must quit. It's got that kind of snappy snare drum beat, but I think it's a more accurate.
Andrew Huberman: So heritability, interacting with experience, filtered through the randomness of development.
David Linden: Yeah.
Andrew Huberman: So we can shorten that up and we'll just call it the Linden hypothesis.
David Linden: I don't think I can take credit for that. It belongs to other people.
Andrew Huberman: Sure. But there's a long history in science of things being shortened up and coined, and that's as important. We're not trying to rob attribution here. And the good news is, perhaps you can't call it the Linden hypothesis, but I can.
David Linden: Alright.
Andrew Huberman: And what I found is, as with the Galpin equation, which is now out there as a hydration, it's a formula that gives broad but research informed parameters as to how much water one should drink in order to maintain proper hydration. For physiologist Dr. Andy Galpin, who's a PhD in physiology and an expert in all aspects of exercise science, there's the Galpin equation, there's the Soeberg principle. So I'm naming these things left and right where appropriate. So I'm naming these things sparingly and where appropriate. So from here on out, heritability, interacting with experience, filtered through the randomness of development, is the Linden hypothesis. And I'll be damned if anyone's going to rename it faster than I'm going to propagate it.
David Linden: All right, well, I think all the geneticists will be gnashing their teeth about this being named after someone who isn't actually a geneticist.
Andrew Huberman: Quite all right. And their dentists will thank me. David Linden : [LAUGHING]
Let's talk about mind-body.
David Linden: Okay.
Andrew Huberman: I'm fascinated by this for a couple of reasons, and I promise to keep this brief. But when I was growing up, I was very interested in animals and biology, and my father's a scientist, and I got very interested in neuroscience early, as people perhaps know. And so much of neuroscience as I was coming up through the mid-90's, 2000's, 2010 to 2020 stretch was focused on the brain piece, very little on the body. There was nothing about gut brain access in the early discussions and coursework. In parallel to all of that, I've been interested in mental health, physical health, and let's just call it performance, and got interested in meditation, respiration based practices, things like Yoga Nidra, things that by way of experience, I understood immediately had a profound influence on the nervous system states of mind and body.
Nowadays, there's an entire institute at the National Institutes of Health for Complementary Health and medicine essentially exploring things like Yoga Nidra, respiration practices, even supplements and things of that sort. And there's this understanding that, oh, my goodness, the nervous system extends into the body, and the body sends neural signals back into the brain. And so this whole notion of mind-body has fortunately migrated away from kind of California counterculture Esalen Institute only hippie, New Age, magic carpet stuff, by the way. That's not what I believe, but that's often how it was looked at in the past. And now people at every level of science and medicine, at every major university and in every scientific journal are starting to publish papers about the interactions between bodily organs, like the breathing apparati, the diaphragm, lungs, the heart, heart rate variability. We hear about the liver, the gut brain axis in particular. And so mind-body, the idea that our thoughts could influence our body and that our bodily state could influence our thoughts is fortunately not just understood, but it seems to be both accepted and appreciated.
So what are your thoughts on mind-body? What does that mean to you? And what do you think is the potential of the mind-body interaction? It seems to me we've just barely scratched the surface.
David Linden: Yeah, well, I'm glad you asked, because I think it's a really fascinating situation and where things are changing very, very quickly. And I think to me, the most important thing for people to understand is that when you have a hypothesis, let's say you have a hypothesis that meditation can attenuate chronic pain. Well, there is a temptation to think that this operates outside the realm of science and biology, that it is in some airy fairy realm in the clouds that this happens for good reason. There are a lot of people who will describe it in exactly that way with auras or they coop scientific terms like resonance and energy, but they don't actually use them in scientific ways. So there's a lot of very fuzzy language that surrounds this. But it shouldn't obscure the fact that when you have a hypothesis that, say, some mental state, like meditation or guided breathing affects some process in the body, that you should be trying to understand this in terms of a biological hypothesis, not in terms of some indistinct realm that is different.
Andrew Huberman: Like manifestation.
David Linden: Yeah, I really learned this initially from my father. My father was a psychiatrist, in fact, kind of a talking cure, old fashioned psychoanalyst had his practice in Los Angeles, and we would have dinner together every Wednesday night, and he would always tell me about his patients. He was very careful to keep confidentiality. He wouldn't break confidentiality, but I would say, oh, yes. How's your narcissist doing? Oh, he had this dream. This was normal conversation when I was 14-15 years old with my dad. And one day I said, dad, it's really clear to me that through this talking cure, a large fraction of your patients feel better and they conquer their depression or their obsessive thoughts or things that are blocking them. How do you think it works? And he says, well, we don't really know the mechanics, but ultimately, when it works, it's not working in some airy fairy realm. It is working by changing the biology of the brain. And when he said that, it was like a lightning bolt went off in my head. And I thought, well, I don't have the kind of personality to be a talking cure psychiatrist. I'm not nearly nice enough, but I could understand the underlying biology. Maybe I'll do that.
And so, as you've correctly pointed out, when you say the phrase mind-body, you're talking about two directions. You're talking about mental functions affecting the body, and then you are also talking about how phenomena in the body affect the mind. And we're understanding so much more about how that happens. And I think the general thing for your listeners to appreciate is that we have some culprits here. Right? And generally speaking, there are two classes of culprits. So if you want to get signals about the body to the mind, there's two ways to do that. One of them is through neurons that reach out into the body and sense things. And this is referred to as interoception. Right? So as opposed to exteroception your outward pointing senses. These are the senses that monitor your own body. And while we can consciously be aware of a lot of that information, a lot of it is happening subconsciously, like your breathing is happening automatically most of the time without you thinking about it. And that depends upon sensors about your blood chemistry and the state of your lungs and a number of other things that are regulating that process. And it's all happening in the brain, usually below the level of your conscious attention.
In addition to the neural signals, there is also a whole realm of hormonal or diffusible immune signals. And what these are is that these are chemicals that are released into the bloodstream and that move throughout the body and that can activate neurons in the brain or in other parts of the nervous system to produce changes in mental function. And I think the real thing that is exciting a lot of people right now has to do with immune signaling molecules. So there's a class of molecules called cytokines. And cytokines are basically the signaling hormones of the immune system, and they can flow through the bloodstream and through lymphatic fluid and reach many parts of the body. We've known for a number of years that the specialized receptors for these cytokines are found throughout the brain, and yet we know very little about what they do. And that's going to be an astonishingly fruitful area of scientific research.
But to give one exemplar, there are a lot of things these days suggesting a link between inflammation in the body, whether it be in the gut or in other places, to depression. Well, how might that work? Well, it could work either through inflammation sensing neurons sending electrical signals to the brain, or, and it's not either or, it could be both. It could be immune signaling, cytokine molecules produced at the site of inflammation that then travel through the bloodstream and the lymphatic system to then reach the brain, bind receptors, and have effects.
And so one of the mysteries about depression is that it's not that tractable to pharmacological therapy. So if you look at people who suffer with depression, about a third of people see significant benefit from modern SSRI and related antidepressant drugs. About a third see very tiny benefit, and about a third see no benefit at all. And part of the reason is because maybe our term depression is too big a bucket. Depression is actually many different biological disorders, and only a subset of those are helped by SSRIs and will need different therapies for the other ones. That's certainly part of it. But part of it might actually have to do with inflammation. So if you think that inflammation is a risk factor in depression, well, you could do something very simple, right? You could gobble an ibuprofen, right? There's a whole bunch of anti-inflammatory drugs that are very well understood. And so, well, what if you just say, all right, let's have a study where we have a bunch of depressed people and we have them all eat anti-inflammatory drugs for a few weeks and we see if this relieves their depression. And the answer seems to be, no, it doesn't. Well, and that's a little bit hard to understand because there are definitely links between inflammation and depression.
So, for example, one of the early treatments for hepatitis C that's since been superseded by more modern drugs was a pro inflammatory cytokine molecule. And when you gave it to people to treat their hepatitis C, almost everyone became depressed on this drug. So I thought, oh, well, this really seems like a link. Likewise, there are certain neurological diseases like multiple sclerosis. It turns out the incidence of depression as a comorbidity in multiple sclerosis is enormous. And you might think, well, there's a trivial reason for that. If you're paralyzed from MS, you're bummed out about life, and that's the reason. But if you look at people who have spinal cord injuries from accidents, they actually have major depression at a rate from people who are uninjured. So that doesn't seem to be it. It's not just that you're bummed out from being paralyzed, although, of course, it's reasonable to be bummed out about being paralyzed, but that's not it. So what happens in MS? Well, there's a bunch of cytokines, including one called interleukin-6, IL-6, that's elevated massively if you take a spinal tap and you look at cerebral spinal fluid. And so that could be causative for depression.
So all these real reasons to think that inflammation is involved, but yet the idea is still a bit messy. So now what if instead of looking at the general population of depressed people, you look at the subset of people that don't respond to SSRI, antidepressants. Are they helped by anti-inflammatories? And there's a bit of a hint that maybe they are. It's not definitive yet. There are a couple of studies. It's right on the edge. But I think this is a really good example of how we are going to see progress very soon in the body to mind part of mind-body medicine that is going to be of enormous benefit to people.
Andrew Huberman: So, interesting. Could I get your thoughts on one candidate hypothesis that I've been thinking about? I've covered depression on a few episodes, and I've had Robin Carhart-Harris from UCSF and Dr. Matthew Johnson from your very own John Hopkins University, both of whom work run laboratories studying psychedelics for the treatment of depression. The clinical trials on psilocybin. And to be clear, psilocybin is still illegal. It's been decriminalized a few places, but we're not talking about recreational use. We're talking about several therapy sessions, and then two without psilocybin, then 2.5 grams, approximately, dosages of psilocybin given separately, again with therapists present, and then follow up therapy sessions, seem to lead to relief of depression in approximately somewhere between 65 and 80% of people. In some cases, total remission, in some cases, some relief without remission.
Okay, so we can kind of set that result on the shelf. It's been repeated a number of different times. Compare that to the results of SSRIs, which seem to help a third of people, a third minimally, and a third not at all. And, of course, there's the side effect profiles of the SSRIs and associated drugs. Not just the SSRIs, bupropion and the other antidepressants that are taken in prescription drug form. And then there's this inflammation piece.
So, could we hypothesize that relief from depression has something to do with neuroplasticity, rewiring of neural circuits, and that psilocybin, we know, can encourage neuroplasticity. And that perhaps SSRIs can encourage neuroplasticity in some people, not all. And that inflammation is a barrier to neuroplasticity. To me, this is the only thing that can reconcile the current status of the results. And then there's ketamine based therapies. And so we have to also kind of set that on the shelf. But let's set that aside on the shelf for now to keep it simple. It seems to me that based on the time course over which SSRIs work, the fact that they increase serotonin very quickly, but the relief from depression comes much later, the fact that neuromodulators like serotonin are intimately involved in neuroplasticity, they can, in some cases, gait neuroplasticity, that it all centers back to changing neural circuits. And so what we're really trying to do, whether or not it's transcranial magnetic stimulation or now we can throw ketamine in there, or psilocybin or SSRIs, that treating depression is about rewiring the brain. It's not about chemical A or B per se, although serotonin seems involved to me. What I'd love to see is, are more studies about the interaction between neuroplasticity and inflammation. And are we seeing that kind of work out there? Because these results sort of sit as disparate, somewhat conflicting, but it seems like inflammation is anti-neuroplasticity. And broadly speaking here, I realize there are many interleukins, some of which are inflammatory, some of which are anti-inflammatory. But is that a meaningful hypothesis? Do you think there's any hope whatsoever to actually cure depression if we sort of start to unify the results in these different camps?
David Linden: Yeah, I think it's a completely reasonable hypothesis. And I would be broader. And I would say, honestly, the relief of any neuropsychiatric condition ultimately is from neuroplasticity in some form or another. And I think it's worthwhile to step back a bit and talk about what neuroplasticity means. To date, there has been a focus on synapses, on the contacts between neurons as the site of neuroplasticity, and that's warranted. Synapses are plastic. They change as a result of experience, as a result of hormone changes, as a result of exercise, as a result of lots of things. But synapses are not the be all and end all of neural function.
So, for example, neurons work by sending electrical signals along their lengths and between neurons and interconverting those with chemical signals. And the processes of generating those electrical signals, the ion channels that are involved, that are embedded in membranes, that are involved in, that are also plastic, they can also change as a result of experience. That's what we call intrinsic plasticity, as opposed to synaptic plasticity.
In addition, there are literal morphological changes. So when we talk about the wiring of the brain, sometimes we're talking about literal wiring. Like cell A wasn't connected to cell B, and now it is, and that changes. And then sometimes, well, actually, cell A was connected to cell B, but cell B wasn't responsive enough. And now there's a change in cell B. So now cell a can fire cell B, and that could have been a result of a change in its synapse, making it more receptive to neurotransmitter release from cell A. Or it could be something intrinsic in cell A that makes it fire its electrical signal, its spike more easily. I think that one of the key cell types. That's going to be important for your hypothesis linking inflammation to synaptic plasticity is going to be a cell called a microglial cell. And microglial cells are non neuronal cells in the brain. They're motile, they can crawl around, they have long processes, and they can gobble things up. They can literally sort of chew away and digest bits of the extracellular scaffolding that surrounds neurons and synapses, and thereby renders them plastic. They can destroy synapses. And there is a lot of indication that certain disease states may involve over exuberant microglia pruning synapses to a degree that they shouldn't. And we know that microglia are chock full of cytokine receptors and so are responsive to inflammatory signals.
When we're talking about inflammation and we're talking about drugs, it's worthwhile to mention that there are a lot of behavioral things that also can influence the signaling. So we know, and I know you've discussed on your program the incredibly salubrious effects of physical exercise on mental function. So exercise is about as good an antidepressant as SSRIs are, and the side effects are only good side effects as opposed to the bad side effects of SSRIs. And again, this isn't working through some airy fairy realm. The reason that exercise works to relieve depression, and the reason that exercise works to maintain your cognitive function as you age is because of biological pathways that we are now uncovering, some of which will involve microglial cells and neurons and other types of cells in the brains, some of which will involve not the neurons in the brain at all, but the brain's vasculature.
So we know that exercise is very salubrious for keeping blood flowing to the brain. And when you're young, you have a superabundance of blood flowing to your brain. So it doesn't matter if it's reduced transiently, you're fine. But as you get older, your blood vessels become more occluded and less elastic, and you're closer to the trouble spot. And if you exercise regularly, you can dilate and make your blood vessels, including those in your brain, more elastic. And that is almost certainly protective against both depression and cognitive decline as we age.
Andrew Huberman: I am a fan of exercise, but I'm fortunate that I enjoy running and some forms of resistance training. So I always assumed that the good side effects were just the positive mood effects until the recent literature that, as you mentioned, improved vasculature, blood flow and reduced inflammation. If not during the exercise, when inflammation actually increases, decreases inflammation. I'm delighted to hear you say the word microglia, and my postdoc advisor, the late Ben Barres, would be especially delighted. People can look up Ben. I'll provide a link to his biography in the show note captions, because he really championed to the point of. I don't know if champion is even a sufficient word. I mean, Ben was beating the drum, saying, we have to pay attention to glia, we have to pay attention to glia. For the longest time, glia were relegated to these other journals. They even had their own journals. And in the last, what is it, ten years, there's been a kind of explosion of research exploring the role of microglia and other glial cell types. And it's really fantastic to see that actually the most abundant cell type in the brain is the glial cell. Are getting the attention they deserve.
David Linden: It's absolutely true. And we scientists like to think that we're very rational creatures and we're not subject to fads, but we totally are, right? When I started out in this field in the early '80s, everything was about opioid peptides, and then there was a period where gaseous neurotransmitters like nitric oxide were all the rage. And right now, glia are in the spotlight for good reason. I'm not trying to say that it isn't worthwhile, but there is this phenomenon of things being fattish and people jumping on bandwagons. And it happens both in terms of the subject we study, but also in terms of the techniques we use. And right now, the technique that is most fattish involves single cell expression profiling, that is, creating a list of what genes are turned on and how strong they're turned on in single cells and seeing how that changes in different cell types and with experience. And it's a very valuable technique. But one could argue that it is perhaps a bit overused and that 15 years from now, people will go back and say, gosh, those folks in 2023 were really overdoing it with the single cell profiling.
Andrew Huberman: And if anyone's thinking about getting into the field of neuroscience or another area of biological or other research, I can just tell you that if you're starting your PhD or your postdoc, take a look at whatever fad is happening now, and just know that in five years it will be something different. And it takes you about five years to finish your PhD or postdoc. So pick something different than what's faddish now, and you'll land right on the money.
David Linden: There's always a lot to do. You don't have to do what everyone else is doing.
Andrew Huberman: And indeed, the deletion test becomes relevant here. The deletion test, as it was described to me by my colleague E.J. Chichilnisky at Stanford, is if you look around and you see one or more groups doing what you want to do very well, just pick something else, your life's going to be a lot more pleasant.
David Linden: Absolutely. I agree with E.J on that entirely.
Andrew Huberman: Let's get back to mind-body. There are a bunch of different domains of mind-body, as you so aptly pointed out, it's bi directional. Mind informs the body, body informs the mind. But we could probably break this down into respiration. So breathing, conscious patterns of breathing, emphasizing inhales or emphasizing exhales, cyclic hyperventilating, etc. Could also be thought patterns, a little bit harder to break those down. But many, not all, but many forms of meditation involve having a very still body. Not all. There's walking meditation, etc, but still body focused mind. Kind of not a state that we are in a lot of times, unless we direct that state. There are still other mind-body patterns of communication through very still body deep relaxation. Things like Yoga Nidra, Non-Sleep Deep Rest. There's hypnosis, there's touch based body mind communication.
If we're going to talk about mind-body also we should refer to as body-mind. How do we dive in and think about this? Because this is involving clearly 1000 different neural pathways, not just the vagus nerve. Typically, people just kind of hang their hat on mind-body must be vagus nerve, and of course it involves the vagus. But the vagus is an extensive set of pathways. So how do you like to frame up mind-body? And what's most intriguing to you about mind-body communication, both in terms of the biology and its practical applications?
David Linden: Well, I think just as we talked about how there are two potential pathways in conveying signals from the body to the mind, there are also two potential pathways, at least two potential pathways to conveying signals from the brain to the body. And they are the neural signals that are conveyed by neurons that actually get there. And they are hormones and neurotransmitters and cytokines that are released from the brain. And I should mention that hormones are actually produced by neurons, including, for example, some of the hormones that you think about as being produced as sex hormones like estrogen is produced by neurons in your brain, for example.
So let me give an example that I think is a bit out there, but I think is really, really fascinating. And this comes from the cancer world. And so melanoma is a bad cancer. It kills a lot of people. It can spread. It's highly metastatic. And we know that melanomas often become innervated. That is to say, they become contacted by neurons and wrapped and receive signals from them. And we also know that if a melanoma becomes innervated, then the prognosis for that patient is worse. It's more likely to grow. It's more likely to spread.
Well, how does that happen? Well, recently, there have been some reports that show that neurons that innervate the melanoma don't act directly onto the tumor cells. Rather, what they do is they secrete a signaling molecule that has a receptor on immune cells that are patrolling the edges of the melanoma tumor and nibbling away at it. And when that signaling molecule is released from the neuron, it shuts down or reduces that immune patrolling function. And then, as a consequence, the tumor can grow and spread and butt off more readily.
Andrew Huberman: So the signal that comes from neurons is sending the ambulances home, so to speak.
David Linden: Yeah, exactly. And the signal is something called calcitonin gene-related peptide or CGRP.
Andrew Huberman: I'm familiar with CGRP from the domain of touch and its involvement in, I think like, itch perception?
David Linden: Pain perception and itch perception, right.
Andrew Huberman: Interesting.
David Linden: Yeah. And so if neurons can affect the progression of cancer through their activity, and these neurons in the periphery are ultimately connected to the brain through a couple of different hops, then is it reasonable to hypothesize that mental processes could affect cancer progression? So let's say we have a hypothesis, and it's a wild hypothesis, and I want to just emphasize that there is not evidence for this, but let's make the hypothesis that says that through meditative practice, you can slow the progression of certain tumors that tend to get innervated. Well, right now, this is just kind of a wild idea, but I think the important thing, as I've said before, is that this is a wild idea with a biological substrate. It's not like you meditate and magic happens and force fields open and the angels sing, and then your tumor shrinks. We are saying that activity in certain areas of the brain is increased by this meditative practice and that this sends signals to this neuron and this neuron that actually go to the tumor and make something happen through this biochemical pathway that we have defined. And to me, this is speculative, but it's also extraordinarily exciting, right? It opens a kind of investigation of mind to body signaling that has received very little attention up to now.
Andrew Huberman: Incredible. And I say incredible because while you're giving an example of CGRP and nerve innervation and metastatic tumors, I absolutely love the idea that a phenomenon involving some practice that could be put under the umbrella of mind-body or body-mind becomes something entirely different when we're trying to hang that on the hook of a biological process. It's like something fundamentally changes.
You know, it's amazing to me, for instance, that early on, psychedelics and breath work, conscious breath work, were lumped together, cost people their jobs at major universities. I won't name the universities because you work at one, I work at another, and there's a third one called Harvard. I guess I just named them. But nowadays, there are laboratories at every single one of those institutions studying deliberate respiration on health, as well as psychedelics and meditation, for that matter, with the goal of understanding what cytokines, what neurotransmitters, etc, change through defined pathways, including vagus, but phrenic nerves and frontal cortex and all the stuff that is considered classic, rigorous neuroscience. So, I do think we've entered a new era. So it's not costing people their jobs anymore. It's actually giving people their jobs, and it's federally funded, which itself is also fantastic, in my opinion. So we are in a new era.
What do you think needs to be done to really nail down the idea that how we think influences our biology, even though it's a total duh, because everyone knows that chronic stress, for instance, can be detrimental, short term stress can actually be beneficial. And stress is a mental process that essentially deploys chemicals in the body that then create other issues in the body, that then create shifts in mental processes. It's so obvious when it's spelled out, but it's just remarkable to me how this has just been lumped in the category of woo science. And I can't quite figure out what needs to be done in order to convince people that their nervous system includes stuff outside the skull and spinal cord. And, of course, it would work this way.
David Linden: Well, right. And I think it is the job of biomedical researchers right now to reclaim a lot of this from the realm of nonsense. And the problem is, there has been a lot of nonsense, and there's sort of a visceral oral reaction when someone says, oh, yeah, well, you can do breath work, and it'll realign your chakras, and that is what will reduce your anxiety or your gut inflammation. And I'm tempted to just go, oh, shut up.
Andrew Huberman: But what if the chakras are collections of nerve innervation of bodily sphincters? And it could make sense, right?
David Linden: It could, but there's got to be some biology. In some cases, these analogies are rooted in something real, and in some cases, they're just made up bullshit. Right? And I think the challenge is to have really rigorous scientific tests of these things, to take it back and to be willing to say, all right, there have been a lot of claims, for example, made about how mental processes can influence the body, and only a subset of those are going to be true. And of that subset, it is our job to understand how they work, both to rationalize them, but also to optimize them and make them better. I mean, there's no question that mental processes affect the body. I mean, we know, for example, that if we just keep you awake and don't let you sleep long enough, you'll die. And what will you die from? You'll die from sepsis, because the barrier between your gut contents and your peritoneum will break down. Rightand Well, so how does that happen? We're just starting to understand. There's a really dramatic example, but there are going to be many more subtle examples.
So you've mentioned breath work a couple of times, and I think this is really interesting. My colleagues who are interested in respiration tell me that you can record in many different places in the brain, many different places in the neocortex and in other regions, and find a signature of the breathing rhythm, sort of as a background to neural activity. You can find it in the cerebellum, you can find it in the frontal cortex, you can find it in the habenula, which is implicated in many things, including depression. You can find it lots of places. So the idea that conscious modulation of your breathing could have manifold effects on neural function I think is reasonable, given that kind of observation.
Andrew Huberman: Hear, hear! Let's talk about you a bit more. You've been so gracious in covering this wide array of topics and with such eloquence, and I must say I've been delighting in all of it. You are in a unique position these days because, if I understand correctly, you've been diagnosed with a fatal illness. I suppose we've all been diagnosed with a fatal illness of sorts, because we're all going to die sooner or later. If you're willing could you tell us the story of how that diagnosis came to be, what your initial reaction was and where things stand now? And perhaps we can explore some of the, well, let's just say pleasant surprises that have emerged since that initial diagnosis.
David Linden: Well, sure, I'd be happy to. So in the summer of 2020, in the dark days of COVID when things were looking really bad, I developed profound shortness of breath. I couldn't get up a flight of stairs without huffing and puffing. And I thought, oh, well, I've got COVID. But I took COVID tests, and they were all negative. But I thought, oh, I must have COVID. I've got the symptoms of COVID I have respiratory issues. I'm feeling weak. It's got to be COVID. And after a while, when this didn't go away, my wife said, look, you got to go into the doctor. This is crazy. You got to find out what's going on. And I did. And they hooked me up to an electrocardiogram, and they said, oh, you've got atrial fibrillation, meaning that your heart is doing two beats every time it should do one. So I have a very high heart rate. And when the heart beats that fast, it can't work very effectively. There isn't enough time to recharge before the next beat comes.
Now, it turns out that there is a very straightforward therapy for this. Atrial fibrillation comes from electrical signaling in the heart, sort of swirling about in a circle and reactivating part of the heart muscle faster than it should. And so if you thread through a catheter in your groin, up through blood vessels, you can put in a little needle and use that to cauterize and to ablate a tiny little strip of cells in the heart that will produce a barrier that will prevent that aberrant return of electrical activity and will cure atrial fibrillation.
So I had that process, that ablation surgery, and sure enough, it cured my atrial fibrillation. I was feeling terrific. And they said, oh, as a follow up, come back a few months later, and we'll do an echocardiogram to see how your heart looks. And they did, and they went, oh, my god. There's this huge mass pressing against your heart. It's like the size of a Coke can. Here's what we think it is. We think it's a hatal hernia. We think your stomach has poked up through the diaphragm muscle, and it's nestling next to your heart. So the way we diagnose this, it's kind of humorous. They say, chug this can of diet Dr. Pepper and then quickly get up on the table, and we'll do the echocardiogram. And in the echocardiogram, we can see a signature of the popping CO2 bubbles in your soda. And if we see those in the mass, then we know it's your stomach.
So I did it. I chugged it. I got up there. Oh, no, it's not your stomach. I said, oh, okay. Well, what we think this is is a teratoma. And a teratoma is a developmental anomaly that you carry, usually from fetal life, where there's a group of different cells that gets in a place where it shouldn't during development and then grows. You've probably heard about people who sometimes get like, a tooth that grows hair in their abdomen. Sometimes women have these around their ovaries, and they're not malignant, they don't spread. It's a fairly easy thing, but I had this enormous Coke can pressing on my heart. It may have, we don't know, have been the source of my atrial fibrillation to start with, but in order to deal with this, I had to have open heart surgery. So I had the surgery, and it was a big, hairy deal. I was told it would last about 5 hours. It turns out it lasted two days.
Andrew Huberman: Wow.
David Linden: They had me on the heart lung bypass machine longer than you're supposed to because you're very likely to throw a clot and get a stroke. Fortunately, that didn't happen. I had very skilled surgeons at Johns Hopkins. It was bleeding so much that they couldn't close the chest, so they had to do all the surgery and then just leave me anesthetized with my chest open until the bleeding stopped. So the surgery was a bear. And then I'm waiting to get the pathology report back on the tissue they removed, and it came back and it was bad news. Sorry. It's not a teratoma. It's not benign. It is a kind of cancer called synovial sarcoma. And synovial sarcoma is a moderately rare cancer, and it usually affects the synovium, which is the lining of the joints or some other places. It's pretty rare to have it happen in the heart. There are a few examples. If you look in the biomedical literature for common cancers like testicular cancer or breast cancer, there are huge tables of statistics from millions of patients on what's been tried and what works and what the prognosis is. For synovial sarcoma of the heart, they're only individual case reports. Oh, there's a guy in Kenya. And he got it. And this is what happened. There's a woman in Minnesota, and this is what happened with her, right? There are no statistics because it's that rare. And the oncologist said, well, I think you've got six to 18 months to live.
Now this was about now 27 months ago. So I've fortunately exceeded that lifespan estimate. And I think we got to be clear also that being oncologist has got to be a terrible job for many reasons. But one of them is that you got to give a lifespan estimate, even if you really don't have the data to do it in a very informed. You can't just say, I won't do it. You got to do it. People expect it. So I'm not saying, like, oh, the oncologist was incompetent because I've outlived my estimate. He was trying to do something based on very little information, made his best guess. So I got this information, and I was furious. I was so angry. Heart cancer? Who the hell gets heart cancer? Is that even a thing? Have you ever heard of somebody with heart cancer?
Andrew Huberman: Not until now.
David Linden: No. Heart cancer? What the f. I've got heart cancer. This is crazy time. I was 59 years old. I got a lot to do. I can't have heart cancer. And what was, I think, transformative for me is that at the same time that I was feeling white hot, angry with the universe, I was also feeling a deep sense of gratitude for what I've had. I've had a terrific life. I'm not that young. I've had a lot of it, and I had great parents, wonderful friends growing up. I've had a good career. It's been a fairly easy run of it. And I think the ability to have a job where you follow your own curiosity every day, there's nothing like that. So few people in the world get to live that way. I feel incredibly grateful, and I feel incredibly grateful for my family. And I have a wonderful wife named Dina, and she's just the best. How do I deserve this? I don't deserve her, you know honestly.
In neuroscience, we often think, oh, well, you have a state, you have a set point. Are you anxious, or are you relaxed? Are you fleeing, or are you approaching? It's like a single axis. Well, but it isn't. And I think most people understand that. But dumb me, I didn't, until that moment, really understand that I could feel profoundly grateful and profoundly angry in the very same moment.
And having cancer and getting the kind of treatments, the chemo, the radiation, it's famously deeply unpleasant. And I had all that. And it was just as unpleasant as anyone's cancer story that you've heard. The radiation burned my esophagus. I couldn't eat for weeks. It was months it was painful to swallow. Bad stuff. Lots of people have had to have bad stuff like this. What I realize it's a deeply unempowering situation to be a medical patient, particularly when there's something serious. You have a limited sense of agency. Things are being done to you. Drugs go in you that make you feel really bad, and there isn't that much to do. And I realized that for me, the sense of agency came from being curious, from being a total nerd about things. And part of what it made me curious about was my own mental processes as they related to my cancer and my cancer diagnosis.
So, for example, I'm getting the chemo. And I should just say, as background, I'm fortunate I don't have a tendency for depression. I'm a pretty upbeat guy. I don't take any credit for that. I think I was just born lucky and raised lucky. Right? But day after day of feeling bad in your body from chemo, boy, it's hard to be positive. It really is. I could not overcome it. My mood got really, really low, and I could tell myself, this is going to be over. It's not going to go on forever. You won't feel this way forever. And you would think that as a rational person, I could talk myself out of that mood, but I couldn't, probably because my brain was awash in Interleukin-6 and I couldn't overcome it. I felt really low. But at the same time, I was sort of out of remove being a nerd about going like, I bet these cytokines are messing me up right now. I bet that's what's going on. And that gave me some sense of agency in a time where otherwise I really wouldn't have it.
Another thing it really brought home to me is this issue that we were discussing earlier about how malleable perception is and perception of time in particular. If someone had said to me when I was healthy before I was diagnosed, you're going to die in five years, I would have gone, oh, no, I'm 59 years old. I should get way more than five years. I got a lot of things to do, professional things, personal things, family things. I got a lot of things to do. No, that wouldn't be right. I'd be very upset. But if you told me after my diagnosis of six to 18 months oh, you get five years? I'd be like, five years? Yeah. That's pretty good. I can do a lot in five years. I can finish up in the lab and I can do some good work, and I can spend time with my family and travel and savor life's pleasures and do all kinds of things. Five years, great. And of course, it's the same five years, right. The only thing that's different is the context.
But I think the thing that really I realized the most is that I really couldn't and still can't engage with the idea of myself being gone. So, yeah, I can do practical things. I can update my will. I can write letters for my people in my lab. So if I kick off, they've got that to take to their next job. I can do all these nuts and bolts things. But in terms of genuinely engaging with my own demise, I really find that as much as I try, I really can't do that. And at first, I thought, well, that's just your own lack of imagination, Linden. It's just know you're not very good at this. But the more I thought about it, I thought, actually, no, this is a human thing. This is a fundamental human thing. And one of the things, when I look back on the 40 plus years I've been doing neuroscience, that's different, is that when I was first trained, the brain was really described as a reactive structure. Something happens in the world. It comes to your sense organs, your eyes, your ears. It goes into your brain it triggers some things. You think. You make decisions, and then you make an action. It goes out to your muscles, and that's the loop, and that's what the brain does. And what we have known in more recent years is that actually, when the brain is waiting for something to happen, it's not just idling and spacing out that the brain is, at every moment, subconsciously trying to predict the near future. Predicting the near future is predicated on the idea that there will be a near future. That is to say that you won't be dead and gone, right? That there'll be a future for you.
And so I think that my ability, which I think is actually a human, I mean, not my ability, my failure, which I think is actually a human failure, to truly engage with my own demise, is a feature. It is a side effect of the fact that the brain is always trying to predict the future. And so that was interesting to me just as a way that my illness was revealing something about the brain. But it also made me think a lot about the world's religions, right? Religion is everywhere in the world. If you ask anthropologists, is there any society that doesn't have religious ideas? They'll say no. They say they don't always have the word religion. They might just say, well, yeah, in this place, everybody knows that the world's on the back of a turtle and that this and this happened. There are these rules. They may not call it religion, but every place in the world has religion. Not everyone is religious, but it is a cross cultural universal. And most religions, not absolutely every single one, but almost every single one, has stories of afterlife or reincarnation, in which your consciousness endures. Well, why would that be? In many religions, they've got a deal, right? Follow these rules in life, and then you'll be rewarded in the afterlife. And that's a very general idea.
Andrew Huberman: Or punished in the afterlife.
David Linden: Or punished in the afterlife. And in some religions, you meld with the divine. In other religions, you're reincarnated as this or that. Heaven or hell. Right? There's variants, but they share that your consciousness endures. And so why is this so popular all over the world? Well, I would hypothesize that it is a side effect of the fact that the brain can't help but always trying to predict the future. When we can't imagine the world without us in it, then we are forced to concoct stories of the afterlife.
Andrew Huberman: Fascinating. And makes me want to ask about this feature of time perception. My undergraduate, graduate, and postdoc advisors all sadly died earlier. Early, really, by pretty much any standard. And I was fortunate enough to be in communication with the last two as they were going through that process. Both of them described a heightened sense of gratitude, especially for things that previously they had not paid attention to. So we call this noticing the little things. But that makes me conclude that something about the knowledge of one's impending death, however far off that might be, shifts our attention, at least temporarily, leads to this sense of slowing down a bit. Because in order to shift our attention to, quote, unquote, the little things or things that we previously overlooked, there's this sense of slowing down. And we know from basic videography photography that slowing down means an increase in frame rate shooting at strobe frame rates gives you the perception of strobe shooting at very high frame rates, allows you to see things in very slow motion. You're noticing subtle variations that normally you overlook.
I'm not trying to be overly reductionist about this process of enhanced gratitude that you describe and how it was alongside intense anger. But have you noticed a shift in your perception of time because you were given initially this, okay, X number of months, and then now with the, you're still here, fortunately, and with this kind of open ended. Well, it wasn't the prediction that was given to you by your oncologist, but it's unclear how long you're going to be here. Which is how most of us exist. You have the sense that it's sooner rather than later, but you don't really know. So I'm curious as to how the idea that, okay, you have twelve months more to live versus more than twelve months, but not infinite. But of course I know that I hopefully have more than twelve months, but it's not infinite. So this idea of the finish line, the cliff, leaving aside whatever might happen afterwards, I don't know, haven't been there. It changes what we notice by way of changing our perception of time. This is a profound tuning of our perception. What are your thoughts on that? And do you notice each sip of coffee? You probably don't notice each step across the kitchen floor in the morning. You're probably paying attention to your lovely wife and kids and things that day. But presumably it's dynamic. But what is your perception of time like now? With the understanding that, yes, you made it through past the gate that was predicted, but what lies ahead is uncertain.
David Linden: Yeah, so that's really interesting. And I would say definitely my perception of time is slower and it seems like an age since I was diagnosed. But I think part of that is because it's been action packed. In other words, since I was diagnosed, so many emotionally salient things, nontrivial things have happened. So many intense discussions with my wife and my friends and the people in my lab. My wife and I have taken a lot more vacations than we normally do. So we're running all over the world and there's a certain sense of packing it in that I think influences time perception. But I would say actually, for me personally, the gratitude isn't about the little things. The gratitude is about the very biggest things. The gratitude is gratitude for being a sentient being and having that blessing. The gratitude is for being able to have a life where I can follow my own ideas and creativity. And my gratitude is for, I choke up, David Linden : [TEARFUL] the profound love that I've felt from my wife and my children.
It's not the little stuff, it's the big stuff that I think about. When I think about gratitude, it's not noticing the sip of tea. It's the big issues and for me, I want to delay my death as much as possible, of course, but when I think about it, the part that makes me upset is leaving people behind. It's not for myself. I've had a great life. I've had a lot of it. I'm 61. I'd like to go longer, but that's a pretty good run. I've gotten to do lots of things in those 61 years and have wonderful, loving, interconnected experiences. And so the negative part is about what I leave behind.
Andrew Huberman: Certainly what you've left behind is enormous and has been the consequence of actions long before your diagnosis, which I think is a clear lesson to everyone. I can't speak for you, but don't wait for the diagnosis. You've mentioned the sense of agency that you felt by being able to pay attention to and explore your experience of, let's call it what it is, impending death. And at the same time, as you mentioned, you've amplified and accelerated the number of things that you've put into the world recently writing incredible articles about your experience of life and death. And we will of course, link to those so people can read them. I've read them all, and they are profound. And they don't just feel important, they clearly are important.
David Linden: Thank you.
Andrew Huberman: So very few people have your insight into the nervous system at the mechanistic level, but also at this more holistic level that you've clearly displayed to us here and in your research and in your book writing and public speaking. I think it's a risky thing to ask somebody for advice, but I can't help myself because I think it's a real opportunity, if you had advice to give to any and all of us based on the whole experience.
David Linden: Yeah.
Andrew Huberman: All of it from go, as I say. If you're willing and feel free to pass. But if you're willing, what is your advice?
David Linden: Well, I would say the advice that is really universal is what everybody already knows and is a bit trite, but I'll say it anyway, and that is appreciate what you got while you got it. And this isn't any big secret and everyone knows it. I would say for a subset of people, the way of the nerd is very empowering. I don't think that's the case for everyone. I think for a subset of people who are deeply curious as their nature, turning that curiosity to your own mortality and your own medical situation can be empowering and useful. But I don't think that should be broad advice. I think that's only for a fraction of people. It's probably the worst thing they could do. And there's nothing wrong with that. Everybody's different, right? Not everyone should adopt the way of the nerd, but for a fraction of people, it's a really good thing to do.
Andrew Huberman: This is normally the portion of a conversation with a guest where I list off the many, many things they've done and how grateful I am. And all of that is absolutely true in the case of you being here today and the work you've done. But I think it's self evident how much you've not just accomplished, but how much knowledge you've put into the world. And not just scientific knowledge, but knowledge about the human experience of others and of yourself. And so I just want to extend a giant thank you on behalf of the listeners and viewers and myself. Thank you for coming here today. Thank you for doing what you do, and so great to still have you here and to have this conversation. And I hope it goes longer. And no matter when it ends, you've done an enormous service to humanity.
David Linden: Well, thank you. That's very kind. It's been a pure pleasure to have this discussion with you.
Andrew Huberman: Thanks, David.
Thank you for joining me today for this discussion with Dr. David Linden. If you're learning from and or enjoying this podcast, please subscribe to our YouTube channel. That's a terrific, zero-cost way to support us. In addition, please subscribe to the podcast on Spotify and Apple. And on both Spotify and Apple. You can leave us up to a five star review. If you have questions for me or comments about the podcast or guests that you'd like me to consider hosting on the Huberman Lab Podcast, please put those in the comments section on YouTube. I do read all the comments.
Please also check out the sponsors mentioned at the beginning and throughout today's episode. That's the best way to support this podcast.
Not on today's podcast, but on many previous episodes of the Huberman Lab Podcast, we discuss supplements. While supplements aren't necessary for everybody, many people derive tremendous benefit from them for things like improving sleep hormone support and focus.
The Huberman Lab Podcast has partnered with Momentous supplements. If you'd like to access the supplements discussed on the Huberman Lab Podcast, you can go to livemomentous, spelled O-U-S. So it's livemomentous.com/huberman, and you can also receive 20% off. Again, that's livemomentous.com/huberman, spelled O-U-S.
If you haven't already subscribed to our Neural Network Newsletter, our Neural Network Newsletter is a completely zero-cost monthly newsletter that includes summaries of podcast episodes as well as protocols, that is, short PDFs describing, for instance, tools to improve sleep, tools to improve neuroplasticity. We talk about deliberate cold exposure, fitness, various aspects of mental health, again, all completely zero-cost. And to sign up, you simply go to hubermanlab.com, go over to the menu in the corner, scroll down to newsletter, and provide your email. We do not share your email with anybody.
If you're not already following me on social media, I am hubermanlab on all platforms. So that's Instagram, Twitter, Threads, LinkedIn, and Facebook. And at all of those places, I talk about science and science related tools, some of which overlaps with the content of the Huberman Lab Podcast, but much of which is distinct from the content of the Huberman Lab Podcast. Again, it's hubermanlab on all social media platforms.
Thank you once again for joining me for my discussion with Dr. David Linden. And last but certainly not least, thank you for your interest in science.
[CLOSING THEME MUSIC]
Join 500,000+ subscribers to get regular emails on neuroscience, health, and science-related tools from Dr. Andrew Huberman.
You'll also get Andrew's exclusive Daily Blueprint. In it, Andrew shares his daily routine. He also shares practical tools and protocols that you can use to stay productive and maximize your health.
By submitting your email to subscribe, you agree to Scicomm Media's Privacy Policy